JLE

Epileptic Disorders

MENU

Charles Bonnet syndrome in hemianopia, following antero-mesial temporal lobectomy for drug-resistant epilepsy Volume 9, issue 3, September 2007

Figures

See all figures

Auteur(s) : Sara Contardi1, Guido Rubboli1, Marco Giulioni2, Roberto Michelucci1, Fabio Pizza1, Elena Gardella1, Federica Pinardi1, Ilaria Bartolomei1, Carlo Alberto Tassinari1

1Neurology Unit, Department of Neurosciences, Bellaria Hospital, Bologna
2Neurosurgery Unit, Department of Neurosciences, Bellaria Hospital, Bologna, Italy

Article reçu le 16 Janvier 2007, accepté le 2 Mai 2007

The Charles Bonnet syndrome (CBS) is a disorder characterized by stereotyped, persistent or repetitive, complex visual hallucinations (such as moving objects or human beings, animals, vivid landscapes), not accompanied by psychotic symptoms, in patients with acquired impairment of vision due to ocular diseases or visual field abnormalities (Bonnet 1769, Schultz and Melzack 1991, Ffytche 2005). Additional diagnostic criteria include full or partial retention of insight into the unreal nature of these phenomena, absence of hallucinations in other sensory modalities, exclusion of substance abuse or sleep disorders (Ffytche 2005). The visual disturbances characterizing CBS can occur for variable periods of time (from days to years), with the hallucinations changing both in frequency and complexity during this time (Schultz and Melzack 1991). Several factors may trigger or stop the hallucinations, presumably through a general arousal mechanism (Kolmel 1985, Schultz and Melzack 1991).Visual field defects, mainly represented by upper quadrantanopias, are a relatively common consequence of anterior temporal lobe resection for refractory temporal epilepsy, with an incidence that can vary from more than 50% (Tecoma et al. 1993) to about 100% (Hughes et al. 1999). In spite of this high incidence of visual field defects, descriptions of CBS in patients who have undergone epilepsy surgery for drug-resistant temporal lobe epilepsy have been reported only rarely. To our knowledge, CBS has been reported in only four patients with upper quadrantanopia following amygdalohippocampectomy (Freiman et al. 2004, Tan and Sabel 2005). In this paper, we describe CBS in an additional patient presenting with right homonymous hemianopia resulting from left antero-mesial temporal lobectomy performed for the treatment of drug-resistant left temporal lobe epilepsy associated with a left temporo-mesial cavernous angioma.

Case report

A 49-year-old, right-handed female reported that, at the age of 35, she had started to experience brief, daily episodes of “déjà-vu” and “jamais-vu”, followed, after few weeks, by two generalized tonic-clonic seizures. Following this, she began to suffer from seizures characterized by an initial epigastric sensation, loss of contact, oro-alimentary automatisms, and repetitive upper limb movements; recovery of consciousness was accompanied by transitory aphasia lasting few minutes. Seizure frequency was two-three seizures per week. Interictal EEG displayed left temporal spikes. Brain MRI showed a cavernous angioma in the mesial portion of the left temporal lobe (figure 1).

She was admitted to the Department of Neurosciences at Bellaria Hospital in Bologna to undergo a presurgical evaluation that included prolonged video-EEG monitoring for seizure recording. She had never suffered from psychiatric disturbances or from disorders of vision; her visual field at admission was normal. She was on phenytoin, 400 mg/day; all previous medications had failed to satisfactorily control her seizures. The results of video-EEG monitoring indicated an epileptogenic area that extended from the left anterior lateral neocortex and temporal pole to the left mesio-temporal region; consequently, a left antero-mesial temporal lobectomy was performed (figure 1).

Immediately after surgery, she presented a transitory aphasic disorder lasting a few days; in addition, she started to complain of blurred vision in her right hemifield. A visual field examination demonstrated right hemianopia (figure 2). On the third day after surgery, she started to experience complex visual hallucinations in her right hemifield, with no impairment of consciousness: she usually referred to seeing coloured “Lilliputian” figures of women and children, either static or moving, but usually running in meadows or even lying in bed with her, or, occasionally, brightly coloured countryside scenes. She was concerned about the possible epileptic nature of these phenomena, although she acknowledged that they were completely different from the “dejà vù” episodes she had previously suffered from. These visual imageries were not accompanied by hallucinations involving other sensory modalities; in particular, no auditory sensations were referred to. She was aware of their fictitious nature and she could describe precisely what she was experiencing, without being frightened; in fact, she was sometimes amused by them. After a while, she noticed that she could make them disappear by closing her eyes. During these hallucinatory episodes, several EEG recordings failed to show any epileptiform abnormalities, but sporadic bursts of theta activity in the left anterior temporal leads, unrelated to the visual imageries, were observed. These phenomena were almost continuous in the first days, but then started to occur intermittently, more often in the evening, lasting from a few seconds to a few minutes. They became progressively less frequent; after two months, they disappeared spontaneously.

Discussion

Although anterior temporal lobectomy for drug-resistant temporal epilepsy is complicated, in a high percentage of cases (from more than 50% to nearly 100%) (Tecoma et al. 1993, Hughes et al. 1999), by post-surgical visual field impairment, reports of CBS in epileptic patients with visual field defects following this surgical procedure are actually rather rare. In the literature, only four patients have been described, by Freiman et al. (2004) and Tan and Sabel (2005): in all of them CBS was associated with upper quadrantanopia following amygdalohippocampectomy for the treatment of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. In the two patients reported by Freiman et al. the visual hallucinations consisted of, in one case, moving geometrical objects, and of unfamiliar, moving human faces restricted to the specific region of the visual field defect, in the other; these phenomena appeared the day after surgery, vanishing after a few days. In our patient, the surgical procedure to remove the cavernoma and the epileptogenic area, caused a complete homonymous hemianopia, presumably due to more extensive damage of the optic radiations beyond Meyer’s loop, the most anterior portion of the optic radiation, and possibly of the optic tract. Complex visual hallucinations in the defective hemifield appeared few days after surgery and persisted, although with decreasing frequency, for about two months. The characteristics of the hallucinatory manifestations fulfilled the criteria for CBS (Schultz and Melzack 1991, Ffytche 2005): in fact, they consisted of complex images, generally women and children, reduced in size (referred to as “Lilliputian” by the patient), brightly coloured and either static or moving; more rarely, panoramic views were perceived. The patient recognized their unreal nature, as was not frightened by them; non-visual hallucinations were never reported.

As reported elsewhere (Kolmel 1985), she could make them disappear, almost at will, by closing her eyes; the effectiveness of eye closing at stopping this kind of visual phenomenon has been suggested to depend upon an arousal mechanism, modulated by eye movements (Kolmel, 1985; Schultz and Melzack, 1991); an alternative explanation suggests that inhibitory impulses originating from the proprioceptors of the extraocular muscles might reach the occipital cortex via superior colliculi, interrupting visual imageries (Kolmel 1985). Of course, a possible epileptic nature was considered; however, epileptic visual hallucinations are usually brief and remarkably stereotypical, whereas in our patient, they could last several minutes and could differ in content from episode to episode. Moreover, visual phenomena in occipital epilepsy exhibit different characteristics, whereas when occurring in temporal lobe seizures, they are most often accompanied by other symptoms or signs (such as, for instance, confusion, mental slowing, clouding of consciousness) (Bien et al. 2000). Finally, in our patient, we never observed epileptiform abnormalities in any of the several EEGs performed during the hallucinatory episodes. Therefore, we concluded that an epileptic origin was extremely unlikely. In table 1, we summarize the main features of the visual hallucinations in epileptic seizures and in CBS.

Considering the high incidence of visual field deficits after antero-mesial temporal resection, it is somewhat surprising that CBS is reported so rarely in epileptic patients who underwent this surgical treatment. Indeed, it is possible that the incidence of CBS is underestimated because most patients suffering from this disturbance are reluctant to complain of it because of the fear of being considered “mentally” disturbed (Freiman et al. 2004). In fact, in some instances it has been reported that the patients admitted that they were experiencing visual hallucinations only when specifically asked (Freiman et al. 2004).

The pathophysiological mechanisms underlying CBS remain incompletely understood. Functional imaging studies in CBS patients have shown that hallucinations of colour, faces, textures and objects correlated with the time course of the fMRI signal in the ventral occipital lobe, with a correspondence between the nature of the hallucinatory phenomena and the specialization of the different visual areas (Ffytche et al. 1998). Regarding the processes causing the appearance of CBS in subjects with acquired abnormalities of visual function, Cogan (1973) postulated that missing inputs to the visual cortices (i.e., a “de-afferentation state”) might trigger the appearance of visual hallucinations by disinhibiting visual association areas through a “release” mechanism. Recently, Ffytche (2005) proposed the existence of two phenomenologic syndromes, one depending directly on visual system pathologies, the other on damage in the brainstem or in ascending neurotransmitter (particularly cholinergic) pathways. The characteristics of CBS in our patient, i.e. self-limiting hallucinations consisting of faces or unfamiliar figures or extended landscapes, lasting seconds or minutes, confined to a particular area of the visual field and progressively diminishing with time, fit with the features of the first syndrome proposed by Ffytche (2005), in which they result from excitation of visual cortical areas through local cortical pathology or secondary to functional/lesional visual deafferentation.

In conclusion, our report adds further evidence of the occurrence of CBS in visual field deficits following anterior temporal lobectomy for the treatment of drug-resistant temporal epilepsy. A proper diagnosis is necessary to reassure patients with post-surgical CBS that the manifestation is not an epileptic disorder and that it usually presents a benign, self-limiting course, not requiring treatment.
Table 1 Main features of the visual hallucinations in epileptic seizures and in CBS.

Visual hallucinations in epilepsy

Visual hallucinations in Charles Bonnet syndrome

Duration

seconds; rarely minutes

from seconds to several minutes

Facilitating or triggering factors

triggered by visual stimuli in occipital/occipito-temporal seizures

facilitated by darkness, or dim light; rarely by light.

Voluntary modulation

no

possible (by eye closure, or saccadic eye-movements)

Visual field defects

no

yes

Contents

  • - elementary hallucinations, illusions and visual loss in occipital/occipito-temporal seizures
  • - elementary and complex hallucinations in temporal seizures


coloured complex hallucinations (moving objects, human beings, animals, vivid landscapes)

Stereotyped

yes

no

Emotional valence

unpleasant, pleasant, neutral

neutral, rarely pleasant

Awareness of unreal nature

sometimes

always

Other symptoms or signs

frequent (consistent with the spread of the epileptic discharge as in occipital, occipito-temporal, temporal seizures)

no

Other sensory hallucinations

possible

no

Ictal EEG

epileptiform abnormalities

normal

Acknowledgements

We thank Mrs Clementina Giardini for her assistance in preparing the manuscript. This study has been partially supported by a grant from the Italian Ministry of University and Research (years 2004-2006).